Attention Research Update
April 2005
"Helping parents, professionals and educators stay
informed about new research on ADHD"
David Rabiner,
Ph.D. Senior Research Scientist, Duke
University
The possibility that diet exerts a significant
influence on ADHD symptoms was proposed over 3 decades ago by Dr. Ben Feingold,
a pediatrician who suggested that eliminating a variety of artificial food
colors (AFCs), naturally occurring salicylates (salicylates are chemicals
that occur naturally in many fruits and vegetables), artificial flavors,
and particular preservatives could substantially reduce ADHD symptoms in
many children. Controlled trials of the Feingold diet (FD) first appeared
in the literature during the 1970s, and by now, a number of qualitative reviews
and one statistical summary of the effects of this diet on ADHD symptoms
have been published. The qualitative reviews have reached mixed conclusions
on the benefits of the Feingold Diet, and the debate between different authors
has frequently been highly contentious. The only statistical summary of the
different studies was published over 20 years ago and concluded that placing
children on the FD had only a small and non-significant affect on ADHD symptoms.
(If you would like to learn more about the Feingold Diet, yu can do so at
www.feingold.org ).
A paper published in a recent issue of
the Journal of Developmental and Behavioral
Pediatrics provides an updated analysis on the role of AFCs in the
behavior difficulties of children with ADHD (Schab & Trinh (2004). Do
artificial food colors promote hyperactivity in children with hyperactive
syndromes? A meta-analysis of double-blind placebo controlled trials. JDBP,
25, 423-434.) The authors of this paper argue that an updated analysis
of this issue is needed for several reasons, noting that the initial statistical
summary was overly broad in the studies it included, and that this could
have reduced the association between diet and ADHD symptoms that was reported.
In addition, several well-conducted studies have been published since that
initial summary.
In the current review, the authors began
by identifying all published studies that reported the results of placebo-controlled
double blind trials on the relationship between the consumption of AFCs (artificial
food colors) and behavioral change in children with ADHD. A total of
15 trials that included a total of 219 participants were identified.
All trials were double-blind cross over
trials which means that during the trial, children received two different
diets, one that included AFCs and one that did not. Children's behavior was
rated by parents, and/or teachers, and/or clinicians while on both diets.
In all cases, those rating children's behavior were unaware of which diet
the child was receiving at the time. This procedure is necessary to
insure that any behavior differences found when children were on the different
diets did not reflect the pre-existing biases of the raters, and is comparable
to what is done in placebo-controlled trials of medication treatment for
ADHD.
In a meta-analysis such as this one, rather
than considering the results of each study separately, the researchers pool
the findings across studies and conduct an analysis that that examines the
overall pattern of results. The benefit of this approach is that it minimizes
idiosyncratic findings of any individual study, and provides a more reliable
basis for drawing conclusions about the overall pattern of findings in a
particular research area.
RESULTS
The primary finding from the meta-analysis
was that children's behavior showed a statistically significant improvement
when AFCs were eliminated from their diet. The size of the improvement
was relatively modest- about a third to a half as large as the improvement
typically associated with medication treatment for ADHD.
The authors next examined whether eliminating
AFCs resulted in greater behavioral improvement for children who were previously
screened for responsiveness to AFCs. This, in fact, was the case.
Children who had demonstrated responsiveness to AFCs through a non-blinded
trial, or whose parents believed they were responsive to dietary factors,
showed a more substantial improvement when AFCs were removed during the double-blind
study. This results suggests that parents are sensitive to whether
their child's behavior is adversely affected by diet, and that if parents
believe this to be true, eliminating AFCs from their child's diet is likely
to result in behavioral improvement.
One important caveat to these findings
is that the impact of eliminating AFCs on children's behavior was only evident
in parents' ratings, and not in the ratings of teachers or clinicians.
Because parents, teachers, and clinicians were all unaware of which diet
children were on when they completed their ratings, this difference cannot
be attributed to preexisting biases on parents' part. However, it does
suggest that although removing AFCs may yield behavior improvements in children
that parents are sensitive to, it is less likely to result in improvements
that teachers observe. This is quite different from what is generally
found in studies of medication treatment or behavior therapy, where teachers
generally report significant improvement in children's functioning.
SUMMARY AND IMPLICATIONS
Results from this meta-analysis provide
strong evidence that the behavior of children with ADHD can be made worse
by dietary factors, and that eliminating AFCs from their diets will, on average,
result in behavioral improvements. This result is consistent with with accumulating
evidence that neurobehavioral toxicity may result from a wide variety of
distributed chemicals.
The authors note that the mechanism by
which AFCs may adversely affect children's behavior is not known, and suggest
that it may occur because of allergic reactions, or because of actual pharmacological
effects that AFCs induce. They also suggest that results from the trials
they analyzed may potentially underestimate the actual effect of AFCs because
several trial employed doses that were well below children's true likely
daily exposure. In addition, because the impact of AFCs on behavior
and learning may occur within several hours of ingestion, too much time may
have elapsed between the administrati0n of AFCs and the measurement of outcomes
to fully capture how participants were affected by ingesting AFCs. Finally,
although not discussed by the authors, the studies included in their meta-analysis
only examined the impact of eliminating AFCs from children's diet, and it
is possible that greater behavioral changes would result from eliminating
other items, e.g., artificial flavors and preservatives, as well.
On the one hand, these results supports
the belief held by many parents that their child's behavior is adversely
affected by dietary factors. As discussed above, this was particularly
true for children whose parents held a prior belief that their child was
sensitive to AFCs. For such children, dietary interventions may thus
play a meaningful role in helping to manage at least some of the behavioral
difficulties associated with ADHD. What percentage of children with ADHD
this subgroup represents is not discussed in the paper, however, and I am
not aware of data on this important question.
Despite these basically positive findings,
the authors also point out several important factors that qualify their results.
First, as noted above, ratings from parents - but not teachers - showed improvement
when AFCs were eliminated from children's diets. This is in marked
contrast to what is typically found with medication treatment. Because improving
children's behavior and learning at school is such a critical issue in ADHD
treatment, this is a very important consideration.
Second, the degree of improvement parents
reported when AFCs were eliminated were smaller than improvements typically
associated with medication. This was true, although to a less extent,
even for those children who were screened for initial responsiveness to AFCs.
Finally, it is important to note that several
trials included in the meta-analysis employed ratings that were not specific
to ADHD symptoms as the indicator of change. Instead, outcomes were
sometimes measured using reports of symptoms that are believed to be specific
to AFC ingestion such as sleeplessness and irritability, and which de-emphasized
restlessness and inattention. Other studies measured child outcome
using scales that were customized for each child based on parents' report
of how their child was affected by diet. Because these were blinded
trials, the use of these alternative outcome measures does not invalidate
the finding that parents, on average, observed improvement in children's
behavior when AFCs were eliminated. It does raise important questions,
however, about whether such improvement will necessarily occur in the core
symptoms of ADHD.
What are the clinical implications of these
findings? First, it is important to note that treatment guidlines from
the American Academy of Pediatrics and the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry both recommend recommend medication treatment and/or
behavior therapy as the current treatments of choice for ADHD. The
authors themselves emphasize the need for additional research and are cautious
about making any clinical recommendations. They note that the restrictive
nature of AFC free diets may place a burden on children and families, and
suggest that until more certain methods have been developed to identify AFC-responsive
children, "imposition of the diet should be done reluctantly".
If parents would like to try an AFC elimination
diet for their child, however, the findings reported in this study suggest
that it may be useful in cases where parents have reason to believe that
their child is sensitive to AFCs. In addition, parents and children
would need to feel comfortable with the restrictions that an AFC free diet
will entail. If this approach is tried, it will be especially important to
carefully monitor whether improvements that parents may observe are also
reported by the child's teacher, as evidence presented above suggests that
behavioral improvements in the classroom are less likely to occur.
Thus, parents cannot assume that their child is doing any better at school
even if they observe improvements in the child's behavior at home.
In situations where children are struggling
in these areas, even if their behavior has improved at home, additional interventions
to address these difficulties will be important to implement. This
may include medication treatment and/or behavioral therapy and/or specific
academic assistance, intervention components that currently enjoy the strongest
empirical support for helping children with ADHD, and which be necessary treatment
components even for those children who derive some benefit from dietary interventions.
Information presented in Attention Research
Update is for informational purposes only, and is not a substitute for professional
medical advice. Although newsletter sponsors offer products and services
that I believe will be of interest to subscribers, sponsorship of Attention
Research Update does not constitute a specific endorsement or guarantee of
any company's product or services.